Premium
This is an archive article published on May 26, 2007

Life, and how to tell it

Researchers have found strong correlations between the content of people’s current lives and the life stories they narrate

.

For more than a century, researchers have been trying to work out the raw ingredients that account for personality. They have largely ignored the first-person explanation—the life story that people themselves tell about who they are, and why.

Stories are stories, after all. The attractive stranger at the airport bar hears one version, the parole officer another, and the PTA board gets something entirely different. Moreover, the tone, the lessons, even the facts in a life story can all shift in the changing light of a person’s mood, its major notes turning minor, its depths appearing shallow.

“When we first started studying life stories, people thought it was just idle curiosity,” said Dan P. McAdams, a professor of psychology at Northwestern and author of the 2006 book,The Redemptive Self. “Well, we find that these narratives guide behaviour in every moment, and frame not only how we see the past but how we see ourselves in the future.”

Story continues below this ad

Researchers have found that the human brain has a natural affinity for narrative construction. People tend to remember facts more accurately if they encounter them in a story rather than in a list, studies find; and they rate legal arguments as more convincing when built into narrative tales rather than on legal precedent.

YouTube routines notwithstanding, most people do not begin to see themselves in the midst of a tale with a beginning, middle and eventual end until they are teenagers. “Younger kids see themselves in terms of broad, stable traits: ‘I like baseball but not soccer,’ “ said Kate McLean, a psychologist at the University of Toronto in Mississauga. “This meaning-making capability—to talk about growth, to explain what something says about who I am—develops across adolescence.”

The researchers found strong correlations between the content of people’s current lives and the stories they tell. Those with mood problems have many good memories, but these scenes are usually tainted by some dark detail. The pride of college graduation is spoiled when a friend makes a cutting remark. The wedding party was wonderful until the best man collapsed from drink. A note of disappointment seems to close each narrative phrase.

By contrast, so-called generative adults—those who score highly on tests measuring civic-mindedness, and who are likely to be energetic and involved—tend to see many of the events in their life in the reverse order, as linked by themes of redemption. They flunked sixth grade but met a wonderful counsellor and made honour roll in seventh. They were laid low by divorce, only to meet a wonderful new partner. Often, too, they say they felt singled out from very early in life—protected, even as others nearby suffered.

Story continues below this ad

Depending on the person, the story itself might be nuanced or simplistic, powerfully dramatic or cloyingly pious. But the point is that the narrative themes are, as much as any other trait, driving factors in people’s behaviour, the researchers say.

“We find that when it comes to the big choices people make—should I marry this person? should I take this job? should I move across the country?—they draw on these stories implicitly, whether they know they are working from them or not,” Dr. McAdams said.

Jonathan Adler, a researcher at Northwestern, has found that people’s accounts of their experiences in psychotherapy provide clues about the nature of their recovery. In a recent study presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology in January, Adler reported on 180 adults from the Chicago area who had recently completed a course of talk therapy. They sought treatment for things like depression, anxiety, marital problems and fear of flying, and spent months to years in therapy.

Adler found that in fact those former patients who scored highest on measures of well-being—who had recovered, by standard measures—told very similar tales about their experiences. They described their problem, whether depression or an eating disorder, as coming on suddenly, as if out of nowhere. They characterised their difficulty as if it were an outside enemy, often giving it a name (the black dog, the walk of shame). And eventually they conquered it.

Story continues below this ad

The findings suggest that psychotherapy, when it is effective, gives people who are feeling helpless a sense of their own power, in effect altering their life story even as they work to disarm their own demons, Adler said.

In a 2005 study reported in the journal Psychological Science, researchers at Columbia University measured how student participants reacted to a bad memory, whether an argument or failed exam, when it was recalled in the third person. They tested levels of conscious and unconscious hostility after the recollections, using both standard questionnaires and students’ essays. The investigators found that the third-person scenes were significantly less upsetting, compared with bad memories recalled in the first person.

“What our experiment showed is that this shift in perspective, having this distance from yourself, allows you to relive the experience and focus on why you’re feeling upset,” instead of being immersed in it, said Ethan Kross, the study’s lead author. The emotional content of the memory is still felt, he said, but its sting is blunted as the brain frames its meaning, as it builds the story.
BENEDICT CAREY (The New York Times)

Latest Comment
Post Comment
Read Comments
Advertisement
Advertisement
Advertisement
Advertisement